Small-group collaboration and individual knowledge acquisition: The processes of growth during adolescence and early adulthood
ARTICLE
Christine Howe, University of Cambridge, United Kingdom ; Antonia Zachariou, University of Roehampton, United Kingdom
Learning and Instruction Volume 60, Number 1, ISSN 0959-4752 Publisher: Elsevier Ltd
Abstract
Research into small-group collaboration during middle to late childhood shows that while individual understanding can be promoted through exchanging differing opinions, the joint analyses that groups construct while collaborating play a tangential role. Individuals may or may not accept these constructions depending upon processes of reflection and reconciliation that are triggered through difference and sometimes occur post-group. Recognizing a dearth of research with older participants (together with inconclusive suggestions that collaborative constructions may become more significant with age), the reported study examines the impact of small-group collaboration during adolescence and early adulthood. Forty-six pairs of students aged between 10 and 22 years worked on a computer-presented task that required them to discuss and predict the trajectories objects follow when they fall from stationary or moving carriers. Associations between group dialogue and post-test performance confirmed a key role for differing opinions while collaborative constructions turned out to have little relevance.
Citation
Howe, C. & Zachariou, A. (2019). Small-group collaboration and individual knowledge acquisition: The processes of growth during adolescence and early adulthood. Learning and Instruction, 60(1), 263-274. Elsevier Ltd. Retrieved February 8, 2023 from https://www.learntechlib.org/p/208151/.
This record was imported from
Learning and Instruction
on March 15, 2019.
Learning and Instruction is a publication of Elsevier.
Keywords
References
View References & Citations Map- Anderson, A., Howe, C., Soden, R., Halliday, J., & Low, J. (2001). Peer interaction and the learning of critical thinking skills in further education students. Instructional Science, 29, pp. 1-32.
- Anderson, A., Tolmie, A., Howe, C., Mayes, J., & Mackenzie, M. (1992). Mental models of motion. Models in the mind: Theory, perspectives and application, pp. 59-71. London: Academic Press.
- Asterhan, C., & Schwarz, B. (2009). The role of argumentation and explanation in conceptual change: Indications from protocol analyses of peer-to-peer dialogue. Cognitive Science, 33, pp. 373-399.
- Azmitia, M., & Montgomery, R. (1993). Friendship, transactive dialogues, and the development of scientific reasoning. Social Development, 2, pp. 202-221.
- Bakhtin, M. (1981). The dialogic imagination: Four essays. Austin: University of Texas Press.
- Barnes, D., & Todd, F. (1977). Communication and learning in small groups. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
- Baron, R., & Kenny, D. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, pp. 1173-1182.
- Berkowitz, M., Gibbs, J., & Broughton, J. (1980). The relation of moral judgment stage disparity to developmental effects of peer dialogues. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 26, pp. 341-357.
- Chi, M.T.H., & Wylie, R. (2014). The ICAP framework: Linking cognitive engagement to active learning outcomes. Educational Psychologist, 494, pp. 219-243.
- Damon, W., & Killen, M. (1982). Peer interaction and the process of change in children's moral reasoning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, pp. 347-367.
- Damon, W., & Phelps, E. (1988). Strategic uses of peer learning in children's education. Peer relations in child development, pp. 135-157. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
- Doise, W., & Mackie, D. (1981). On the social nature of cognition. Social cognition: Perspectives on everyday understanding, pp. 53-83. London: Academic Press.
- Doise, W., & Mugny, G. (1979). Individual and collective conflicts of centrations in cognitive development. European Journal of Psychology, 9, pp. 105-108.
- Doise, W., & Mugny, G. (1984). The social development of the intellect. Oxford: Pergamon.
- Eckstein, S., & Kozhevnikov, M. (1997). Parallelism in the development of children's ideas and the historical development of projectile motion theories. International Journal of Science Education, 19, pp. 1057-1073.
- Eckstein, S., & Shemesh, M. (1989). Development of children's ideas on motion: Intuition vs. logical thinking. International Journal of Science Education, 25, pp. 671-688.
- Felton, M., & Kuhn, D. (2001). The development of argumentive discourse skills. Discourse Processes, 32, pp. 135-153.
- Field, A. (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS Statistics. Los Angeles: Sage.
- Fung, D., & Howe, C. (2014). Group work and the learning of critical thinking in the Hong Kong secondary liberal studies curriculum. Cambridge Journal of Education, 44, pp. 245-270.
- Hattie, J.A. (2008). Visible learning. London: Routledge.
- Higgins, S. (2013). Self-regulation and learning: Evidence from meta-analysis and from classrooms. British Journal of Educational Psychology Monograph Series II, 10, pp. 111-126.
- Howe, C. (2009). Collaborative group work in middle childhood: Joint construction, unresolved contradiction and the growth of knowledge. Human Development, 39, pp. 71-94.
- Howe, C. (2010). Peer dialogue and cognitive development: A two-way relationship?. Educational dialogues: Understanding and promoting productive interaction, pp. 32-47. London: Routledge.
- Howe, C., Devine, A., & Tavares Taylor, J. (2013). Supporting conceptual change in school science: A possible role for tacit understanding. International Journal of Science Education, 35, pp. 864-883.
- Howe, C., McWilliam, D., & Cross, G. (2005). Chance favours only the prepared mind: Incubation and the delayed effects of peer collaboration. British Journal of Psychology, 96, pp. 67-93.
- Howe, C., & Mercer, N. (2007). Children's social development, peer interaction and classroom learning. The Primary Review (Research Survey 2/1b). Cambridge: University of Cambridge.
- Howe, C., Rodgers, C., & Tolmie, A. (1990). Physics in the primary school: Peer interaction and the understanding of floating and sinking. European Journal of Psychology of Education, pp. 459-475.
- Howe, C., Tavares Taylor, J., & Devine, A. (2012). Everyday conceptions of object fall: Explicit and tacit understanding in middle childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, pp. 351-366.
- Howe, C., & Tolmie, A. (2003). Group work in primary school science: Discussion, consensus and guidance from experts. International Journal of Educational Research, 39, pp. 51-72.
- Howe, C., Tolmie, A., Anderson, A., & Mackenzie, M. (1992). Conceptual knowledge in physics: The role of group interaction in computer-supported teaching. Learning and Instruction, 2, pp. 161-183.
- Howe, C., Tolmie, A., Duchak-Tanner, V., & Rattray, C. (2000). Hypothesis testing in science: Group consensus and the acquisition of conceptual and procedural knowledge. Learning and Instruction, 10, pp. 361-391.
- Howe, C., Tolmie, A., & Rodgers, C. (1992). The acquisition of conceptual knowledge in science by primary school children: Group interaction and the understanding of motion down an incline. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10, pp. 113-130.
- Howe, C., Tolmie, A., & Sofroniou, N. (1999). Experimental appraisal of personal beliefs in science: Constraints on performance in the 9 to 14 age group. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, pp. 243-274.
- Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking. New York: Basic Books.
- Jurkowski, S., & Hänze, M. (2015). How to increase the benefits of cooperation: Effects of training in transactive communication on cooperative learning. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, pp. 357-371.
- Kapur, M. (2008). Productive failure. Cognition and Instruction, 26, pp. 379-424.
- Krist, H. (2000). Development of naïve beliefs about moving objects: The straight-down belief in action. Cognitive Development, 15, pp. 281-308.
- Kruger, A. (1992). The effect of peer and adult-child transactive discussions on moral reasoning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38, pp. 191-211.
- Kuhn, D. (1989). Children and adults as intuitive scientists. Psychological Review, 96, pp. 674-689.
- Kuhn, D. (1991). The skills of argument. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Kuhn, D., Garcia-Mila, M., Zohar, A., & Andersen, C. (1995). Strategies in knowledge acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60, p. 245.
- Larrain, A., Freire, P., & Howe, C. (2014). Science teaching and argumentation: One-sided versus dialectical argumentation in Chilean middle-school science lessons. International Journal of Science Education, 36, pp. 1017-1036.
- Leman, P., & Duveen, G. (1999). Representations of authority and children's moral reasoning. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, pp. 557-575.
- Littleton, K., & Mercer, N. (2012). Communication, collaboration, and creativity: How musicians negotiate a collective ‘sound’. Musical imaginations. Multidisciplinary perspectives on creativity, performance and perception, pp. 233-241. New York: Oxford University Press.
- Marioni, C. (1989). Aspects of students' understanding in classroom settings (age 10-17): Case study on motion and inertia. Physics Education, 24, pp. 273-277.
- McCloskey, M. (1983). Naïve theories of motion. Mental models, pp. 299-324. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
- Mercer, N., & Littleton, K. (2007). Dialogue and the development of children's thinking: A sociocultural approach. London: Routledge.
- Miell, D., & Littleton, K. (2004). Collaborative creativity. London: Free Association Books.
- Miell, D., & MacDonald, R. (2000). Children's creative collaborations: The importance of friendship when working together on a musical composition. Social Development, 9, pp. 348-369.
- Mollard, D. (2009). Moral development, social interaction, gender and the co-ordination of perspectives. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. UK: University of Cambridge.
- Mugny, G., & Doise, W. (1978). Socio-cognitive conflict and structure of individual and collective performances. European Journal of Social Psychology, 8, pp. 181-192.
- Piaget, J. (1959). Judgment and reasoning in the child. Michigan: Littlefield Adams.
- Piaget, J. (1985). The equilibration of cognitive structures: The central problem of intellectual development. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
- Preacher, K.J., & Hayes, A.F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 36, pp. 717-731.
- Roseth, C.J., Johnson, D.W., & Johnson, R.T. (2008). Promoting early adolescents' achievement and peer relationships: The effects of cooperative, competitive, and individualistic goal structures. Psychological Bulletin, 134, pp. 223-246.
- Roy, A., & Howe, C. (1990). Effects of cognitive conflict, socio-cognitive conflict and imitation on children's socio-legal thinking. European Journal of Social Psychology, 20, pp. 241-252.
- Sampson, V., & Clark, D. (2008). The impact of collaboration on the outcomes of scientific argumentation. Science Education, 93, pp. 448-484.
- Schauble, L. (1990). Belief revision in children: The role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, pp. 31-57.
- Schwarz, B., Neuman, Y., & Biezuner, S. (2000). Two wrongs may make a right….If they argue together!. Cognition and Instruction, 18, pp. 461-494.
- Silverman, I., & Geiringer, E. (1973). Dyadic interaction and conservation induction: A test of Piaget's equilibration mode. Child Development, 44, pp. 815-820.
- Slavin, R.E., & Lake, C. (2008). Effective programs in elementary mathematics: A best-evidence synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 78, pp. 427-515.
- Tolmie, A., & Howe, C. (1993). Gender and dialogue in secondary school physics. Gender and Education, 5, pp. 191-209.
- Tolmie, A., Howe, C., Mackenzie, M., & Greer, K. (1993). Task design as an influence on dialogue and learning: Primary school group work with object flotation. Social Development, 2, pp. 183-201.
- Van der Stel, M., & Veenman, M.V. (2014). Metacognitive skills and intellectual ability of young adolescents: A longitudinal study from a developmental perspective. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 29, pp. 117-137.
- Vogel, F., Kollar, I., Ufer, S., Reichersdorfer, E., Reiss, K., & Fischer, F. (2016). Developing argumentation skills in mathematics through computer-supported collaborative learning: The role of transactivity. Instructional Science, 44, pp. 477-500.
- Von Aufschnaiter, C., Erduran, S., Osborne, J., & Simon, S. (2008). Arguing to learn and learning to argue: Case studies of how students' argumentation relates to their scientific knowledge. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 45, pp. 101-131.
- Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
- Vygotsky, L. (1998). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky. Volume 5: Child psychology. New York: Plenum Press.
- Whitaker, R. (1983). Aristotle is not dead: Student understanding of trajectory motion. American Journal of Physics, 51, pp. 352-358.
- Williams, J., & Tolmie, A. (2000). Conceptual change in biology: Group interaction and the understanding of inheritance. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18, pp. 625-649.
- Winne, P.H., Hadwin, A.F., & Perry, N.E. (2013). Metacognition and computer-supported collaborative learning. The international handbook of collaborative learning, pp. 462-479. New York: Taylor & Francis.
These references have been extracted automatically and may have some errors. Signed in users can suggest corrections to these mistakes.
Suggest Corrections to References